Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them (1)

Link to Publisher's site

PLoS One. 2023; 18(7): e0288451.

Published online 2023 Jul 26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0288451

PMCID: PMC10370735

PMID: 37494357

Rebekah Mifsud, Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editingWorldviews and the role of social values that underlie them (2)1,* and Gordon Sammut, Writing – review & editing2

Peter Karl Jonason, Editor

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Associated Data

Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement

Abstract

In today’s ideologically diverse world, it is pertinent to have a better understanding of how our beliefs of the social world shape our thinking and behaviour. The purpose of this paper is to investigate the key social values that underlie particular sets of beliefs, referred to here as worldviews. Worldviews encompass beliefs that shape one’s outlook on life and are, therefore, instrumental in providing meaning to one’s reality and one’s understanding as to how one fits in it. They can be classified into five unique types, namely, Localised, Orthodox, Pragmatist, Reward, and Survivor. In this paper we start by proposing a theoretical relationship between this five-factor typology and social values. Following this, we present findings that show that worldviews may be mapped onto the two higher order value dimensions of Openness to Change versus Conservation, and Self-transcendence versus Self-Enhancement. We conclude by outlining the implications that these findings have on understanding individual cognition and society in general.

Introduction

Investigating beliefs naturally solicits the question of what purpose they serve. Beliefs exist at varying levels of generalizability and are shaped and reinforced by culture, experience, and theology [1]. For this reason, they serve multiple purposes drawing upon the need to form “enduring, unquestioned ontological representations of the world” [2]. When a set of related beliefs combine, they do so in terms of overlapping substantive content or shared functionality. Either way, when they do they form belief systems that, when coherently clustered, are recognisable as generalized worldviews or ideologies. The understanding of belief systems and how different beliefs bind together has been a relatively popular focus of social research [35]. From political ideologies to religious beliefs, various studies have suggested that belief systems need not just be contained within the individual but rather may also exist across individuals [6], facilitating ways of developing alliances with others, maintaining a shared reality, and extending the lifespan of the belief system beyond the believers themselves [1, 3]. It follows, therefore, that belief systems have an important role in both personal identity and society, serving the psychological needs of the individual as well as the institutionalised power structures of society. More relevant to our study is the need to understand how elements of belief systems, such as values [1], play a role in shaping and informing our understanding of ourselves and the world around us, and in what manner they serve to guide our actions. The need to investigate this is evident when considering the highly divided world we live in. For instance, religious beliefs and political ideologies are known to exert a significant influence on social cohesion [7]. Accordingly, it is not unreasonable to hypothesise that worldviews too may play such a role. Investigating the values that underlie our worldviews is an essential step towards understanding the motives and perspectives of individuals that enable effective communication and collaboration amongst diverse actors.

In this paper, we start by defining worldviews and values and follow with foraging a theoretical linkage between the two. We then proceed by reporting the findings of a study of this theoretical overture before we conclude with a discussion of implications and suggestions for future study. We conclude by asserting that the study of worldviews offers a pathway towards the understanding of coalitional and oppositional projects undertaken by social groups in everyday life.

Worldviews

Beliefs and belief systems share several essential properties and features, namely: they vary in generalizability and strength; they may arise and be reinforced by experience, culture, society, philosophy, and theology; they are instrumental in helping us provide meaning of the world, ourselves and our place in society; and they also share a strong relationship with behaviour [1]. It is reasonable, therefore, to propose that we are wired for storing beliefs and using them to navigate the world around us. Buhagiar and Sammut [8] explain how beliefs serve an extended dual purpose of describing elements in our environment for the purpose of guiding action. Similar sentiments have been subsequently proposed by Power et al. [9] with regards to world-making. This involves worldviews, that is, a particular set of generalized beliefs that we use to describe ourselves and the world around us. Worldviews encompass beliefs that shape our outlook on life and they are pertinent in providing meaning to our reality and our understanding of how we fit within it [10]. In an extensive review of the literature on worldviews, Koltko-Rivera [10] has pointed out that the construct of worldviews has oftentimes been defined and named in a multitude of ways, from cultural and value orientations aimed at conceptualizing worldviews at a social level [1113], to philosophical outlooks aimed at conceptualizing worldviews at an individual level [14]. More than a decade later, this scenario on worldviews remains largely the same, lacking a unified understanding of the concept. We attempt to remedy this pitfall in the present paper. Similar to Koltko-Rivera [10], we define worldviews as representations of the structure of how and what people think. We propose that their function lies in how they operate to enable subjects to adapt their responses to present ecological demands [15, 16]. As outlined by Sammut et al. [16], a number of theoretical constructs fall in line with this definition of worldviews that have employed different terms. Sammut et al. (2022) identify four theories that propose remarkably identical five-factor typologies, namely: (i) symbolic universes [17], (ii) social axioms [18], (iii) moral foundations [19], and (iv) deep stories [20]. It is worth noting that these typologies possess various similar features. Firstly, they do not solely focus on individual disposition but situate individual dispositions within the wider social sphere, tapping into psychological constructs that shape the way individuals interpret their social world. Secondly, they all serve the practical purpose of enabling individuals to adapt suitably to different situational demands. For instance, the symbolic universe, Interpersonal Bond; the moral foundation, Loyalty/Betrayal; and the deep story profile, Team Player, emphasize pro-social behaviour. Conversely, the symbolic universe, Others’ World; the social axiom, Social Cynicism; the moral foundation, Authority/Respect; and the deep story profile, Cowboy, emphasize selfish behaviour. With consideration to these commonalities, Sammut et al. [16] proposed the notion of worldviews, offering a novel five-factor typology aimed at unifying the above-mentioned concepts. The five worldview types include the (i) Localised, (ii) Orthodox, (iii) Pragmatist, (iv) Reward, and (v) Survivor worldviews [16]. As summarised in Table 1, each worldview captures a symbolic universe, social axiom, moral foundation, and a deep story profile. The Localised worldview involves the desire to fix problems or address social issues. The Orthodox worldview seeks to preserve the status quo. The Pragmatist worldview is protective and revolves around self-interest. The Reward worldview centres around determination to work hard to obtain a desired goal. Lastly, the Survivor worldview involves fatalism, distrust in others and the need to overcome adversity [16]. It is worth noting that what differentiates these worldviews from the other similar five-factor typologies is the way in which they are measured, namely through vignettes. Vignettes are better suited for identifying worldviews because they provide a rich holistic formulation that may otherwise not be captured through the sum of a sequence of Likert scales. Specifically, the worldview vignettes offer a flexible approach in which, given a narrative, respondents are allowed to formulate and consider a generalized situational outlook when interpreting them [21]. For instance, when interpreting a Survivor worldview, a respondent in India might be despairing about food whilst a respondent in the USA might be despairing about mortgages. Ultimately, despite the differences in personal experiences that respondents draw upon, the psychological experience remains similar. Therefore, worldviews can be thought to be the phenomenological filter for engaging the cognitive miser, acting as a lens through which individuals interpret their own personal experiences.

Table 1

A syncretic conceptualisation of worldviews as proposed by Sammut et al [16].

WorldviewsSymbolic UniversesSocial AxiomsMoral FrameworksDeep Stories
LocalisedInterpersonal BondSocial ComplexityLoyalty/BetrayalTeam Player
OrthodoxOrdered UniverseReligiosityPurity/SanctityWorshipper
PragmatistNiche of BelongingnessFate ControlFairness/ReciprocityRebel with a Cause
RewardCaring SocietyReward for ApplicationHarm/CareCosmopolitan
SurvivorOthers’ WorldSocial CynicismAuthority/RespectCowboy

Social values

Belief systems and values are linked to each other because the former allows the manifestation of the latter [1, 22]. One could argue that the distinction resembles that between genotype and phenotype in evolution. Values are formulated on the basis of what an individual or social group deems to be important, desirable or favourable, playing a key role in bridging the gap between individual and society [13, 23]. One of the most established theories of values is the one outlined by Schwartz [13]. In his theory Schwartz [13] defines values as individually held subjective beliefs that (a) are strongly associated with feelings (b) refer to desirable goals that motivate action (c) are ordered in level of relative importance, and (d) set a standard on which judgements and decisions are made. Furthermore, values are also defined as universal because they are thought to satisfy three universal requirements of human existence, namely, the needs of individuals as human beings, of harmonious social interaction, and of survival and welfare of social groups [23]. Schwartz’s theory of values organizes them in the form of a circumplex consisting of 10 broad value types, namely: (1) self-direction (independent thought and action), (2) stimulation (excitement towards life), (3) hedonism (gratification for oneself), (4) achievement (personal success), (5) power (authority and status), (6) security (safety and stability), (7) conformity (following social norms), (8) tradition (respecting customs), (9) benevolence (well-meaning towards others), and (10) universalism (respecting of all people and nature) [13, 23]. Empirical evidence for this model emerged from smallest space analysis that examined the spatial relationships amongst the values [13]. Notably, Schwartz et al. [24] have recently developed a more detailed value circumplex consisting of 19 different value types that can, however, be collapsed into the original 10. Additional analysis of the original 10 value circumplex revealed a two-dimensional structure [13, 23]. On the one hand, Conservation versus Openness to Change reflects the tension between values relating to preservation or change of the status quo [13]. On the other hand, Self-Enhancement versus Self-Transcendence reflects the tension between values relating to personal or other-related interests and successes [13]. Schwartz’s theory has been validated across a wide range of countries and cultures, and the measures of these values (i.e., the Schwartz Values Survey [SVS]), has demonstrated strong psychometric properties [2426]. In addition, meaningful relationships have been reported between values and beliefs [13, 14].

The hypothesized relationship between worldviews and values

Worldviews and values may be thought of as comprising a hierarchical structure with values being the more abstract and worldviews being the less abstract [27]. Earlier it was noted that values are universal and applicable regardless of context [23]. Particularly, since values transcend specific situations and contexts, they offer an opportunity to understand the motivational constituents that make up one’s avowed worldview. The values that we refer to in our study are the higher order values identified by Schwartz [13, 23]. Though Schwartz’s value theory and the concept of worldviews are distinct frameworks having different foci and applications, we believe there is a degree of correspondence between the two. Particularly, the two higher order values outlined seemingly correspond with Triandis’ dissection of individualism-collectivism dimensions, which have been linked to cultural worldviews [28, 29]. Due to this, Schwartz’s higher order value dimensions, that are more individual-oriented, offer an opportunity to link values to our conceptualisation of worldviews. Indeed, with reference to Schwartz’s value theory, empirical evidence has provided support for a meaningful relationship between individually-held beliefs and generalized values [30, 31]. For instance, Feldman [30] reported that despite evidence that values and moral foundations are unique and separate constructs, findings still indicate a telling relationship between the two. In one contrast, the Harm/Care and Fairness/Reciprocity foundations were associated with higher benevolence and universalism values when collapsed under the higher order value of self-transcendence. In the other contrast, the Loyalty/Betrayal, Authority/Respect, and Purity/Sanctity foundations were associated with higher tradition, conformity, and security values when collapsed under the higher order value of conservation [30]. These findings corroborate an earlier meta-analysis examining the value-attitude relationship based on moral foundation theory [32], where self-transcendence values were found to be related to the Fairness foundation/pro-environmental attitudes and the Care foundation/pro-social attitudes [33]. Conversely, conservation values were found to be related to the Purity foundation/religious attitudes and the Authority foundation/political attitudes [33]. With reference to research on social axioms and values, Social Complexity has been reported to positively correlate with self-direction and benevolence values, Reward for Application has positively correlated with conformity values, and Fate Control and Religiosity have positively correlated with tradition values [31]. Though such studies do not directly tap into the construct of worldviews being investigated here, they are meaningful in their implications on the construct (see Table 1). For this reason, such findings offer a strong basis for predicting a relationship between worldviews and values (see Table 2 for summary of predictions).

Table 2

Summary of worldview descriptions and predictions.

WorldviewSummary DescriptionDominant Supporting Values
LocalisedPositive outlook to people and the world, contributing towards the wellness of others.Self-Transcendence Openness to Change
OrthodoxWilling to contribute to the wellness of others without the desire to change the status quo.Self-Transcendence Conservation
PragmatistWilling to bend the rules for loved ones to navigate an unfair world.Self-Enhancement Openness to Change
RewardHard working with a strong drive for achievement, prioritising in-group over others.Self-Enhancement Conservation
SurvivorFatalistic and cynical view of people and the world.Conservation

The Localised worldview is associated with a generally positive outlook of people and the world, with a strong willingness to contribute towards the wellness of others. Furthermore, it is also associated with flexible and open views. As outlined in Table 1, this worldview is conceptually linked to the Social Complexity social axiom, amongst others. Social Complexity has been positively linked to values of self-transcendence [31]. Furthermore, prosocial behaviour, the central underlying characteristic of the Localised worldview, has also been linked to values of self-transcendence. In light of these findings, it is reasonable to expect that the Localised worldview will correlate positively with values of self-transcendence. Findings relating these beliefs to the values of openness to change, or conservation, are not entirely in synch. Specifically, the Loyalty/Betrayal foundation has been linked to values of conservation, however, the Social Complexity social axiom has not been linked to either of the values of conservation or openness to change. These noncomplementary findings may be attributed to slight variation in each belief’s underlying notions, or perhaps even to differing methodological approaches. Nevertheless, considering that the Localised worldview is conceptually linked to open mindedness, it is expected to positively correlate with values of openness to change. These expected linkages emphasize the significance of other-related interests and the resistance of maintaining a status quo for the Localised worldview.

The Orthodox worldview is associated with a generally positive outlook of people and the world, however, without the desire to change the status quo. For this reason, this worldview is characterised by rather rigid and convergent thinking, ready to accept the current state of matters with little challenge. Indeed, in a study on views towards recreational cannabis use, Sammut et al. [16] reported that the Orthodox worldview stood out from the other worldviews in predicting opposition towards recreational cannabis use. The Orthodox worldview is conceptually linked to the Purity/Sanctity foundation and the Religiosity social axiom, both of which were correlated with values of conservation [30, 31, 33]. Furthermore, Religiosity was also found to be positively linked to values of self-transcendence [31]. With consideration to these findings, it is expected that the Orthodox worldview correlates positively with values of self-transcendence and conservation.

The Pragmatist worldview is associated with distrust in social institutions and a relatively negative outlook of people and the word. Despite this, individuals who endorse the Pragmatist worldview also believe that one can easily navigate such a world if one is willing to adapt and bend the rules. This worldview is conceptually linked to the Fairness/Reciprocity foundation and Fate Control social axiom, amongst others. It is worth noting that findings on the two belief systems are different, namely, the Fairness/Reciprocity foundation positively correlates with values of self-transcendence whereas Fate Control does not [30, 31, 33]. Another conceptual link to this worldview includes the Niche of Belongingness symbolic universe. Salvatore et al. [17] claim that this symbolic universe, along with Interpersonal Bond, may be seen as a source of bonding social capital (i.e., prioritizing in-group identity and cohesion). For this reason, it will be expected that, like Fate Control but unlike the Fairness/Reciprocity foundation, this worldview will correlate negatively with values of self-transcendence (and so positively with values of self-enhancement). Additionally, due to the element of distrust in those with power, it is expected that the Pragmatist worldview will correlate positively with values of openness to change. In an early study investigating the relationship between values and trust in institution, Devos et al. [34] reported that levels of trust correlated positively with values that emphasize security, preservation, and tradition, that is, those values subsumed under the higher order value of conservation. The authors also reported that levels of trust correlated negatively with values that emphasize change and independent action, that is, those values subsumed under the higher order value of openness to change. These findings were later corroborated by Morselli et al. [35] through a multilevel assessment carried out on cross-cultural datasets.

The Reward worldview is largely associated with hard work and a strong drive for achievement. Importantly, it is also characterised by obedience and respect of social norms. For this reason, an individual endorsing this worldview believes that life’s consequences are generally always fair and deserved, especially if one is unable to exercise restraint over their actions that violate the status quo. The Reward worldview is conceptually linked to the Harm/Care foundation and the Reward for Application social axiom, amongst others. Findings linking the two beliefs to values have shown a positive link between the Harm/Care foundation and values of self-transcendence [30, 33], and between the Reward for Application social axiom and values of conservation [31]. As outlined earlier, the Harm/Care foundation represents the notion of looking after others. Although this aspect is shared with the Reward worldview, it is worth noting that possibly, for the Reward worldview, caring for others may arise as a by-product of the desire to be in a higher position (i.e., a parental/authority figure). Therefore, it is possible that the Harm/Care foundation is rooted in more egalitarian intentions in contrast to the Reward worldview. Due to this conceptual difference, it is expected that unlike the Harm/Care foundation, the Reward worldview positively correlates with values of self-enhancement. Furthermore, due to characteristics relating desire for authority and control to prevent harm, and in line with the findings on the Reward for Application social axiom, the Reward worldview is expected to correlate positively with values of conservation.

Lastly, the Survivor worldview is associated with a fatalistic and cynical view of people and the world. This negative view is also accompanied by significant distrust in society and its institutions. The Survivor worldview is conceptually linked to the Authority/Respect foundation which was found to correlate positively with values relating to conservation [30]. In addition, it is conceptually linked to the Social Cynicism social axiom which has correlated positively with the value of power but not with other values collapsed under the higher order values of self-transcendence and self-enhancement [31]. For this reason, it is not theoretically evident how the Survivor worldview correlates with the self-transcendence/self-enhancement value tension. Nevertheless, considering the findings within the domain of moral foundations [30] and findings linking fatalism to the values of conservation [36], it can be reasonably expected that the Survivor worldview correlates positively with conservation values.

Method

This study formed part of a larger exercise investigating the cognitive and behavioural correlates of different beliefs, and their influence on self-regulatory processes. For the present purposes, only methods, data analyses, and results pertaining to the component investigating the correlations between worldviews and values will be reported.

Participants and procedure

Participants were recruited through Prolific Academic (ProA). ProA has been reported to produce superior data quality for behavioural research when compared to other online recruitment platforms such as Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) [37]. Since the study was a multi-part study, entailing participation in two different sessions, participants were pre-screened in ProA using the criteria of having already taken part in a minimum of five other studies and possessing an approval rate of 100%. These criteria were selected to ensure that participants had prior experience with using ProA and taking part in online studies.

Participants were provided with an online information letter and consent form outlining the details of the study. Following consent of participation by clicking the “continue to experiment” button, the first session commenced. This session contained the worldviews scale and vignettes, amongst other measures. Once the first session was completed and after a few hours had elapsed, the second session was made available to the same participants. This session contained the PVQ-RR, amongst other sessions. Each session lasted around 20 minutes and participants were rewarded a total of £7.00. This study received self-assessed ethical clearance following the University of Malta’s research code of ethics and ethical clearance procedures.

An initial total of 290 participants were recruited, 33 of which failed the attention checks put in place to ensure good quality of the data, and 6 participants failed to participate in the second session, resulting in 251 participants. Out of the 251 participants (Mage = 25.12, SDage = 3.20), 156 identified as female and 95 identified as male. All participants resided in one of the OECD countries as per ProA’s sign-up criterion. Most participants resided in Europe (66.9%), followed by Africa (17.9%), America and Canada (8.4%), and lastly Australia (6.0%).

Measures

Worldviews

A list of 5 vignettes were used, each characterising one of the worldviews (see Table 3). The vignettes were created and eventually refined through a preliminary study in which a correlation analysis was carried between items used in the measures of symbolic universes [17], social axioms [18], moral foundations [19], and the initial conceptualisation of the worldview vignettes. For each of these vignettes, participants were asked to rate the extent to which they believed that each vignette applied to themselves using a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). In addition, participants were also required to select a single vignette that best approximated their own views. This measure of worldviews has reliably been used in a different study that investigated the role of worldviews in predicting support for recreational cannabis use [16].

Table 3

Worldview measures.

WorldviewVignette
LocalisedThe future depends on us and the choices we make. Every problem has a solution. Each and every one of us can make an effort to fix the laws and institutions so that they can be just and equal for everyone. Like this we can better address the needs of people and society.
OrthodoxTo succeed in life, we need to follow the rules and local customs in order to maintain social order. We also need to show respect to each other and carry out our duties. Like this we can help others in our community.
PragmatistIn life we must adapt ourselves to our circ*mstances and sometimes we need to go with the flow in order to avoid trouble. The rich and powerful protect their own interests, whereas the kind-hearted suffer. Sometimes you have to work around the rules to help your loved ones.
RewardIn life, you get what you deserve. Life’s challenges are overcome with the efforts we make, and these may offer new opportunities. One must co-operate with others, respect authority, and carry out one’s duties. Our efforts will eventually lead to success.
SurvivorIn life, things rarely end up well. People are what they are, and good people usually suffer and are exploited. It is best for one to keep his/her head down and get on with it.

Social values

The Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ-RR) is an alternative to The Schwartz Value Survey (SVS) that is more suitable for online administration. It comprises 57 short verbal portraits that describe a person’s goals and aspirations, implicitly tapping into a particular value. There are 3 verbal portraits for each of the 19 values. Typically, all portraits are gender-matched with the respondent, however, for the present study, gender neutral pronouns (they/their) were used to facilitate the online administration of the questionnaire. Participants were asked to complete the PVQ-RR by indicating the extent to which they believe they are like the person being described in each of its portraits, using a 5-point Likert scale. A 5-point Likert scale was adopted to match it with other scales being used in the questionnaire. PVQ-RR allowed for the measure of the 10 personal values, originally identified by Schwartz [13], the refined 19 values [24], and 4 higher order values [24]. The questionnaire demonstrates good psychometric properties for measuring personal values in non-clinical groups [24]. Particularly, the mean Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities for the 4 higher values in the present sample were: .88 self-transcendence, .79 self-enhancement, .84 openness to change, and .80 conservation.

Data analyses

The 19 values, outlined by Schwartz [23], were calculated by taking the average rating across the 3 verbal portraits that are related to the particular value [32]. Following this, the 4 higher order values were calculated as follows: self-transcendence was calculated by computing the mean score of the values of universalism and benevolence; self-enhancement was calculated by computing the mean scores of the values of achievement and power; openness to change was calculated by computing the mean score of the values of self-direction and hedonism; and conservation was calculated by computing the mean score of the value of security, tradition, and conformity [38]. As per the PVQ-RR scoring and analysis instructions recommended by Schwartz [38], centred value scores were calculated to correct for scale use bias. This was especially recommended since the primary modes of analyses for this study was correlation analyses and linear regression. Scores were centred for all values by calculating the mean rating across all items (MRAT) and subtracting this from each of the value scores [38].

To examine differences between worldviews, dummy variables for each worldview were created. Separate dummy regression analysis were then carried out including all the dummy worldviews, with the Localised worldview as the reference category and each individual higher order value as the dependent variable. To control for known sex differences and cultural differences in value orientation [39, 40], all analyses entailed a two-block hierarchical model. With reference to cultural differences, since participants resided in a disproportionate variety of countries, these were grouped together in terms of continent. In the [39, 40]two-block hierarchical model, gender (male, female) and continent (Africa, America, Australia, Europe) were included in the first block and the dummy coded worldviews were included in the second block. No issues of collinearity were detected in any of the analyses.

Results

When asked to select the single worldview that best approximated their own, the majority chose the Localised worldview (45.4%), followed by Pragmatist (24.7%), Reward (13.1%), Orthodox (8.8%), and lastly, Survivor (8.0%). When asked to rate the extent of their agreement with each individual worldview, the Localised worldview received the highest rating (M = 4.08, SD = .79) whereas the Survivor worldview received the lowest rating (M = 2.60, SD = 1.17). With reference to value orientations, self-transcendence tended to be scored highest by those preferring the Localised worldview (M = .54, SD = .38) and lowest by those preferring the Reward worldview. (M = .27, SD = .31). Furthermore, self-enhancement tended to be scored the highest by those preferring the Reward worldview (M = -.36, SD = .38) and lowest by those preferring the Orthodox worldview (M = -.76, SD = .54). As for openness to change, it tended to be scored the highest by those preferring the Pragmatist worldview (M = .37, SD = .32) and lowest by those preferring the Survivor worldview (M = .15, SD = .46). Finally, conservation tended to be scored the highest by those preferring the Survivor worldview (M = -.12, SD = .36) and lowest by those preferring the Localised worldview (M = 3.27, SD = .54). These findings are illustrated in Fig 1A and 1B.

Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them (3)

A. Mean (centred) score for self-transcendence and self-enhancement grouped by worldview. B. Mean (centred) score for openness to change and conservation grouped by worldview.

Correlations between worldviews and values

An overview of the correlations between the ratings for each individual worldview and the scores for each of the four higher-order values is outlined in Table 4. The Localised worldview correlated positively with self-transcendence, r = .17, p = .009, and negatively with self-enhancement, r = -.17, p = .008. In contrast, the Reward and Survivor worldviews correlated negatively with self-transcendence, r = -.19, p = .003 and self-enhancement, r = -.21, p < .001 respectively. The Reward worldview also correlated negatively with openness to change, r = -.15, p = .018, and positively with conservation, r = 18, p = .004. Similarly, the Orthodox worldview correlated negatively with openness to change, r = -.15, p = .019, and positively with conservation, r = 19 p = .003. Lastly, the Pragmatist worldview was the only worldview to correlate negatively with conservation r = -.13, p = .040.

Table 4

Variable123456789
1. Self-Transcendence a-
2. Self-Enhancement a-.627**-
3. Openness to Change a-.022.106-
4. Conservation a-.290**-.308**-.668**-
5. Localised.165**-.166**-.018.039-
6. Pragmatist-.030.084.112-.130*.058-
7. Orthodox-.033-.108-.148*.188**.226**.057-
8. Reward-.189**.104-.149*.180*.186**-.005.292**-
9. Survivor-.208**.122-.107.077-.066.204**-.084-.005-
M.416-.562.315-.2364.083.723.422.892.60
SD.354.555.388.369.794.9851.0261.2201.174

a Centred value scores

*p < .05.

**p < .01

Regression analyses

To investigate the influence of worldviews on higher order values (see Fig 1A and 1B), a series of hierarchical regression models, with each of the four higher order values as the dependent variable, was carried out. As noted earlier, the first block of the model analysed the influence of the demographic variables (gender, continent) whereas the second block analysed the influence of worldviews.

The model predicting self-transcendence was significant, R2 = .186, F(9, 241) = 3.719, p < .001. In this model, the worldviews alone contributed to 11.5% of the variance. Relative to the Localised worldview, all but the Orthodox worldview resulted in lower self-transcendence: the Reward, β = -.261, t(241) = -4.037, p < .001, Survivor, β = -.181, t(241) = -2.191, p = .029, and Pragmatist worldview, β = -.263, t(241) = -5.072, p < .001, predicted lower self-transcendence scores.

The model predicting self-enhancement was significant, R2 = .96, F(9, 241) = 2.844, p < .01. In this model, the worldviews alone contributed to 6.7% of the variance. Relative to the Localised worldview, the Reward, β = .302, t(241) = 2.834, p = .005, and Pragmatist worldview, β = .283, t(241) = 3.304, p < .001, predicted higher self-enhancement scores.

The model predicting openness to change was significant, R2 = .112, F(9, 241) = 3.372, p < .001. In this model, the worldviews alone contributed to 4.6% of the variance. Relative to the Localised worldview, the Reward, β = -.167, t(241) = -2.272, p = .024, and Survivor worldview, β = -.228, t(241) = -2.418, p = .016, predicted lower openness to change scores.

Lastly, the model predicting conservation was also significant, R2 = .131, F(9, 241) = 4.030, p < .001. In this model, the worldviews alone contributed to 4.3% of the variance. Relative to the Localised worldview, the Reward, β = .160, t(241) = 2.310, p = .022, Survivor worldview, β = .201, t(241) = 2.258, p = .025, and Orthodox worldview β = .191, t(241) = 2.326, p = .021, predicted higher conservation scores.

Discussion

The present paper examined the relationship between worldviews and values. Correlational analysis and a series of hierarchical linear regressions were carried out to assess this relationship as well as the influence that worldviews exert on specific higher order values. The findings show that, even though some belief systems, referred to herein as worldviews, are evidently different from each other, there are nevertheless unique points of convergence that may notably be attributed to underlying values. Moreover, when compared to the results of the regression analyses, the weaker results from the correlation analyses lend further support to the notion that, in quantitative research, the worldviews construct is best suited for explaining a proportion of variance that may otherwise remain unaccounted for by the predictor variables [10].

The Localised and Orthodox worldviews both agree on the value of self-transcendence but disagree on the value of conservation. Therefore, an individual who endorses either of these two worldviews is likely to be someone who subscribes to an egalitarian view of the world motivated to go beyond selfish desires to help and connect with others. However, what differentiates these two worldviews is the extent to which one is willing to act autonomously and freely. Those who endorse a Localised worldview are open to independence and are unrestricted by the need to abide by social order, whereas those who endorse an Orthodox worldview are more self-restricting and more comfortable acting within the confines of tradition and society. Though not empirically investigated, this difference between the two might be attributed to the sense of religiosity or belief in higher supremacy that characterises the Orthodox worldview. Earlier, the Orthodox worldview was proposed to be conceptually linked to the Ordered Universe symbolic universe, the Religiosity social axiom, the Purity/Sanctity moral foundation, and the Worshipper deep story profile. Notably, these have all been described as involving an underlying religious notion [1720]. It could, therefore, be the case that the Orthodox worldview is linked to conservatism due to the tendency to adhere to religious teachings and the security that comes with that, undermining an element of agency and self-direction.

Like the Orthodox worldview, the Reward and Survivor worldviews also value conservation. The Reward worldview has been conceptually linked to the caring society symbolic universe, the Reward for Application social axioms, the Harm/Care moral foundation, and the Cosmopolitan deep story profile. A common feature underlying these beliefs is the importance of forming coalitions, developing trust, and living peacefully with others [1720]. A reason for the link between the Reward worldview and conservation may arise out of the desire of maintaining peace within one’s group. For this worldview, it is possible that such peace is thought to be best achieved by exercising control and establishing and adhering to group norms. On a different note, the Survivor worldview, that has been conceptually linked to fatalistic and cynical beliefs, may be linked to conservation because of a sense of powerlessness. That is, even though one is distrustful of society, one would rather let matters remain as they are rather than risk having to adapt to something new. Put simply, for the Survivor worldview, “it is better the devil you know than the angel you do not know”. Unlike the Orthodox worldview, the Reward and Survivor worldviews do not value self-transcendence. Rather, the Reward worldview, in particular, has been linked to self-enhancement. Self-enhancement represents personal focus and self-protection. However, for the Reward worldview, the positive link with self-enhancement is not necessarily solely highlighting self-serving motives but could, more fittingly, be highlighting the importance of ingroup unity over outgroup helping. Therefore, for this worldview, ingroup favouritism may also explain a positive link with self-enhancement. The Survivor worldview is associated with cynicism. Early studies found empirical evidence linking cynicism with lower self-esteem and lower levels of interpersonal trust [41]. Such negative portrayals of the self and others may be a possible cause that explains why the Survivor worldview devalues self-transcendence and is not particularly linked to self-enhancement.

Similar to the Reward worldview, the Pragmatist worldview also has a negative relationship with self-transcendence and a positive relationship with self-enhancement. The Pragmatist worldview was earlier conceptually linked to Niche of Belongingness symbolic universe, the Fate Control social axioms, the Fairness/Reciprocity moral foundation, and the Rebel with a cause deep story profile. An underlying theme of these beliefs is a preference for individual autonomy coupled with reciprocal favouritism [1720]. Essentially, the Pragmatist worldview utilises the “tit-for-tat” strategy to navigate the world. This strategy, which is synonymous with reciprocal altruism [42], is based on the principle that one reciprocates the other’s actions, collaborating only with individuals who are willing to return the favour [43]. The “tit-for-tat” strategy is an essential survival mechanism because it helps to protect self-interest whilst living peacefully with others [43]. It could be the case that the Pragmatist worldview is linked with self-enhancement because their actions are primarily driven by selfish intentions despite seeming to be altruistic in nature. The Pragmatist worldview potentially presents itself as a good example of how, ultimately, reciprocal altruism is rooted in a self-serving agenda [44].

The conceptual link between the Pragmatist worldview and openness to change differentiates it from the Reward worldview. A reason for this could be that individuals who endorse the Reward worldview find security in their social group whereas those with a Pragmatist worldview do not. Earlier, the Pragmatist worldview was related to a negative view of people and society, making them less likely to depend on others. This in turn makes individuals who endorse this worldview more likely to think and act independently, offering an explanation as to why one would be less willing to act within societal constraints.

Conclusion and future directions

The objective of this inquiry constitutes a starting point for understanding how worldviews may play a role in the formation of coalitions for action [8]. Specifically, the empirical relationship between worldviews and values facilitates the understanding of how individuals may come together and agree to support a cause or a course of action despite clear and widespread intra-group differences. In the pursuit of any cause, some stand to agree for one reason whereas others may agree with the cause or ends pursued for quite different reasons. We propose, therefore, that such agreement involves the coalition of worldviews. In other words, a worldview can ally with another worldview in the pursuit of conservative projects. This would be the case, for instance, in an alliance forged between those holding a Reward worldview and others holding an Orthodox worldview. That coalition, however, may well crumble should self-enhancing versus self-transcendent projects rise to the fore, at which point, the Reward worldview will find an ally in Pragmatist worldviews whilst the Orthodox emerge as a common opponent. This simple example illustrates the potential of understanding worldviews in explaining shifting coalitional dynamics in contemporary political landscapes.

A second domain of inquiry that requires empirical effort concerns the endorsem*nt of worldviews and their cognitive correlates. A key question that arises in this theoretical formulation is whether worldviews are marked by individual differences in cognition that result out of inherent dispositions that incline some individuals towards a worldview more strongly than others, or whether, as Sammut [15] proposed, the worldview repertoire is accessible to all individuals with its utility exclusively contingent on situational circ*mstances. Sammut [15] proposes that individuals are able to change worldviews to ensure adaptation should their life conditions change. In this way, an individual pursuing a Reward worldview may, following a series of unfortunate events, emerge with a Survivor worldview that enables that individual to face adversity with grit even though there may be little to no personal gain. In essence, human subjects equipped with more or less similar cognitive power or prowess, as it were, should not be inclined one way or another relative to any particular worldview. Such inclinations should accrue solely as a consequence of life circ*mstances. Empirical study is required to determine whether this is indeed the case or whether, by contrast, the endorsem*nt of worldviews is underlined by individual differences in cognition that incline people in determined directions.

A third domain of inquiry concerns the influence of educational attainment on one’s worldview. A higher level of education is known to act as a catalyst for expanding knowledge, engaging in critical thinking [45], increasing tolerance towards diverse others [46], and facilitating political and civic engagement [47]. Owing to this, for instance, one might expect that a higher level of education may predispose individuals towards a Localised worldview. Individuals with a higher level of education may be more motivated to address social issues due to the fact that they are exposed to diverse perspectives and are aware of the range of social issues that may accompany them. It would be worth exploring whether this is the case and, if so, to what extent does education play a role in worldviews when compared to other factors such as socioeconomic background.

A final domain of inquiry that emerges from the above concerns is how malleable the endorsem*nt of worldviews might be over time. Developmentally, some people face certain circ*mstances at birth that may be markedly different from those faced by different others, predisposing them to a particular worldview over another. Consequently, one wonders whether worldviews change in the face of changing life circ*mstances and what processes govern such adaptation. For instance, one could determine whether adverse life events like divorce or job loss could nudge individuals towards a Survivor worldview. Developmental/longitudinal research is required to potentially inform the helping professions and their ability to prescribe psychological remedies in the form of changing outlooks in line with more adaptive worldviews considering the individual’s own circ*mstances. In this light, it will be worth looking at the role played by certain demographics in the endorsem*nt of worldviews and the extent to which this endorsem*nt may be a function of grand ecological circ*mstances that mark generational eras. For instance, the study of worldviews stands to be informative in understanding differences between pre- to post-Covid mentalities that may go on to mark the perceptions and expectations of generations to come.

Supporting information

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

References

1. Usó-Doménech JL, Nescolarde-Selva J. What are Belief Systems?Found Sci.2016;21: 147–152. doi: 10.1007/s10699-015-9409-z [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Connors MH, Halligan PW. A cognitive account of belief: a tentative road map.Front Psychol. 2015;5: 1588. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01588 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. van Prooijen JW, Kuijper SMHC. A comparison of extreme religious and political ideologies: Similar worldviews but different grievances.Pers Individ Dif.2020;159: 109888. doi: 10.1016/J.PAID.2020.109888 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Homer-Dixon T, Maynard JL, Mildenberger M, Milkoreit M, Mock SJ, Quilley S, et al. A Complex Systems Approach to the Study of Ideology: Cognitive-Affective Structures and the Dynamics of Belief Systems. Journal of Social and Political Psychology. 2013;1: 337–363. doi: 10.5964/JSPP.V1I1.36 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Sartori G. Politics, Ideology, and Belief Systems.Am Polit Sci Rev. 1969;63: 398. doi: 10.2307/1954696 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Wagner W, Hayes N. Everyday Discourse and Common Sense: The Theory of Social Representations.Basingstoke. Macmillan Education UK; 2005. [Google Scholar]

7. Cuevas JA, Dawson BL. An Integrated Review of Recent Research on the Relationships Between Religious Belief, Political Ideology, Authoritarianism, and Prejudice.Psychol Rep.2021;124: 977–1014. doi: 10.1177/0033294120925392 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Buhagiar LJ, Sammut G. ‘Social Re-presentation for…’: An Action-Oriented Formula for Intergroup Relations Research.Front Psychol.2020;0: 352. doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2020.00352 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Power SA, Zittoun T, Akkerman S, Wagoner B, Cabra M, Cornish F, et al. Social Psychology of and for World-Making.Personality and Social Psychology Review.2023. doi: 10.1177/10888683221145756 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Koltko-Rivera ME. The Psychology of Worldviews.Review of General Psychology.2004;8: 3–58. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.1.3 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Hofstede GH. Culture’s consequences: International differences in world-related values. SAGE; 1984. Available: https://books.google.com/books/about/Culture_s_Consequences.html?id=Cayp_Um4O9gC [Google Scholar]

12. Schwartz S.A Theory of Cultural Value Orientations: Explication and Applications.Comparative Sociology.2006;5: 137–182. doi: 10.1163/156913306778667357 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Schwartz SH. Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries.Adv Exp Soc Psychol.1992;25: 1–65. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Maslow AH. Cognition of Being in the Peak Experiences. 2012;94: 43–66. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1959.10532434 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Sammut G. Mentalities and Mind-Sets: The Skeleton of Relative Stability in Psychology’s Closet.Eur J Psychol.2019;15: 421–430. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v15i3.2103 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Sammut G, Mifsud R, Brockdorff N. The Role of Worldviews in Predicting Support for Recreational Cannabis.Front Psychol.2022;0: 2987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.880537 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Salvatore S, Fini V, Mannarini T, Veltri GA, Avdi E, Battaglia F, et al. Symbolic universes between present and future of Europe. First results of the map of European societies’ cultural milieu. PLoS One. 2018;13: e0189885. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189885 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Leung K, Bond MH. Psychological Aspects of Social Axioms: Understanding Global Belief Systems.Leung K, Bond MH, editors. New York: Springer-Verlag New York; 2009. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-09810-4 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Jonathan Haidt. The righteous mind: why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York, NY: Pantheon Books; 2012. Available: https://books.google.com/books/about/The_Righteous_Mind.html?id=U21BxGfm3RUC [Google Scholar]

20. Hochschild AR. Strangers in their own land: Anger and mourning on the American right. New York, NY: New Press; 2016. Available: https://books.google.com/books/about/Strangers_in_Their_Own_Land.html?id=86v0CwAAQBAJ [Google Scholar]

21. Sammut G, Gaskell G. Points of View, Social Positioning and Intercultural Relations.J Theory Soc Behav.2010;40: 47–64. doi: 10.1111/J.1468-5914.2009.00422.X [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

22. Bond MH, Leung K, Au A, Tong K-K, Chemonges-Nielson Z. Combining social axioms with values in predicting social behaviours.Eur J Pers. 2004;18: 177–191. doi: 10.1002/per.509 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Schwartz SH. An Overview of the Schwartz Theory of Basic Values.Online Reading in Psychology and Culture Article.2012;2. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1116 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Schwartz SH, Cieciuch J, Vecchione M, Davidov E, Fischer R, Beierlein C, et al. Refining the theory of basic individual values.J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;103: 663–688. doi: 10.1037/a0029393 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Piurko Y, Schwartz SH, Davidov E. Basic Personal Values and the Meaning of Left-Right Political Orientations in 20 Countries.Polit Psychol.2011;32: 537–561. doi: 10.1111/J.1467-9221.2011.00828.X [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Schwartz SH, Boehnke K. Evaluating the structure of human values with confirmatory factor analysis. J Res Pers. 2004;38: 230–255. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00069-2 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Homer PM, Kahle LR. A Structural Equation Test of the Value-Attitude-Behavior Hierarchy.J Pers Soc Psychol.1988;54: 638–646. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.638 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Triandis HC, Bontempo R, Villareal MJ, Asai M, Lucca N. Individualism and Collectivism: Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Self-Ingroup Relationships.J Pers Soc Psychol.1988;54: 323–338. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.323 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Triandis HC. The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts.Psychol Rev.1989;96: 506–520. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Feldman G.Personal Values and Moral Foundations: Examining Relations and Joint Prediction of Moral Variables.2020;12: 676–686. doi: 10.1177/1948550620933434 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Leung K, Au A, Huang X, Kurman J, Niit T, Niit KK. Social axioms and values: A cross-cultural examination.Eur J Pers. 2007;21: 91–111. doi: 10.1002/PER.615 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Haidt J, Joseph C. The Moral Mind: How five sets of innate intutions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In: Carruthers P, Laurence S, Stich S, editors. The Innate Mind.New York: Oxford University Press;2008. pp. 367–391. doi: 10.1093/ACPROF:OSO/9780195332834.001.0001 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Boer D, Fischer R. How and when do personal values guide our attitudes and sociality? Explaining cross-cultural variability in attitude-value linkages. Psychol Bull.2013;139: 1113–1147. doi: 10.1037/a0031347 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Devos T, Spini D, Schwartz SH. Conflicts among human values and trust in institutions. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2002;41: 481–494. doi: 10.1348/014466602321149849 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. Morselli D, Spini D, Devos T. Human Values and Trust in Institutions across Countries: A Multilevel Test of Schwartz’s Hypothesis of Structural Equivalence.Surv Res Methods.2012;6: 49–60. doi: 10.18148/SRM/2012.V6I1.5090 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Schwartz SH, Sagie G. Value consensus and importance a cross-national study.J Cross Cult Psychol. 2000;31: 465–497. doi: 10.1177/0022022100031004003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. Peer E, Brandimarte L, Samat S, Acquisti A. Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research.J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;70: 153–163. doi: 10.1016/J.JESP.2017.01.006 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Schwartz ShalomH. Coding and analyzing PVQ-RR data (instructions for the revised Portrait Values Questionnaire).2016. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.35393.56165 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Schwartz SH, Rubel T. Sex differences in value priorities: Cross-cultural and multimethod studies.J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;89: 1010–1028. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.1010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Magun V, Rudnev M, Schmidt P. Within- and Between-Country Value Diversity in Europe: A Typological Approach.Eur Sociol Rev. 2016;32: 189–202. doi: 10.1093/ESR/JCV080 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Singelis TM, Hubbard C, Her P, An S. Convergent validation of the Social axioms survey.Pers Individ Dif. 2003;34: 269–282. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00043-0 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

42. Trivers RL. The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism.Source: The Quarterly Review of Biology.1971;46: 35–57. [Google Scholar]

43. Axelrod R, Hamilton WD. The evolution of cooperation. Science. 1981;211: 1390–1396. doi: 10.1126/science.7466396 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Dawkins R.The selfish gene. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]

45. Huber CR, Kuncel NR. Does College Teach Critical Thinking?A Meta-Analysis.2016;86: 431–468. doi: 10.3102/0034654315605917 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Hainmueller J, Hopkins DJ. Public Attitudes Toward Immigration.Annual Review of Political Science. 2014;17: 225–249. doi: 10.1146/annurev-polisci-102512-194818 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Chan RY. Understanding the purpose of higher education: An analysis of the economic and social benefits for completing a college degree. Journal of Education Policy, Planning and Administration.2016;6: 1–40. [Google Scholar]

  • PLoS One. 2023; 18(7): e0288451.
  • »
  • Decision Letter 0

2023; 18(7): e0288451.

Published online 2023 Jul 26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0288451.r001

Peter Karl Jonason, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

13 Jun 2023

PONE-D-23-05362Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie themPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Mifsud,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. First allow me to apologize for the delay. I struggled to find qualified and willing reviewers for this paper. Second (and thankfully) they both felt the paper is nearly ready, just requiring some minor revisions. Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 28 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office atgro.solp@enosolp. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

3. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

5. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1:Yes

Reviewer #2:Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1:This was a very interesting paper that contributes to the theorisation and empirical study of socio-cognitive mechanisms of worldviews/values. It was wonderful to see that you try to advance a more situational understanding of the concept, which is lacking in the field. I have some minor comments that I hope can be helpful to improver your wonderful work.

1. Even though you clarify the similarities between worldviews and the other concepts are, it is not very clear what are the differences between them and how the idea of the worldview is a better choice. Is it possible to explain a bit more?

2. You are highlighting the importance of situational demands in the conceptualisation and operationalisation of worldviews. Yet it is not entirely clear how this is reflected in the operationalisation of the construct i.e. in the vignettes. For example, who are these ‘others’ in the scenarios, who constitutes an in-group/out-group can vary across situations and topics/projects of interest. You introduce this idea towards the conclusion but it is important to clarify what situational aspects (if any) the vignettes are taking into account. Another point is that you are drawing on Schwartz’s values which are quite abstract and do not take into account the importance of situations and contexts. Can you please explain address this?

3. Can you please explain a bit more why you chose for your study Schwartz’s values as opposed to the other constructs you review in your introduction?

4. In the sampling section, can you please provide some more information about the educational background of the participants? Did you also control for that in your analyses? If not, it's ok. Can you just please explain why.

5. In the results section, some of the correlations are quite weak. Can you please comment on that and what implications this has for your theorisations and findings?

6. In the results section, why did you choose to perform the analyses with the higher order values eg self-transcendence and not the individual ones e.g. benevolence, universalism ? There has been some evidence that goes against Schwartz’s value relationships. Can you explain your rationale?

7. In terms of further research and reflections, the vignettes seem to capture rather secular and anthropocentric worldviews. What about relationships to more than human worlds eg nature, spirits, but also orientations to time (role of temporality- how we understand history links to our worldviews maybe Lola Olufemi could be an inspiration?). Another question is: Does a worldview include what is or also what could be different? Perhaps it would be interesting to engage with questions about speculation and world-making e.g. Power, S. A., Zittoun, T., Akkerman, S., Wagoner, B., Cabra, M., Cornish, F., Hawlina, H., Heasman, B., Mahendran, K., Psaltis, C., Rajala, A., Veale, A., & Gillespie, A. (2023). Social Psychology of and for World-Making.Personality and Social Psychology Review,0(0).https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683221145756

8. In your conclusion, you argue about the role of situations and you bring the example of poor versus rich people. You argue ‘poverty may predispose individuals towards a Survivor worldview whilst riches might predispose others towards a Reward worldview’ Actually this is an erroneous belief. There is a lot of evidence to the contrary. You can refer to literature on development eg Robert Chambers, Arturo Escobar

Congratulations on a super interesting paper and for making a case for the importance of situations in the study of worldviews.

Reviewer #2:please see attached document--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1:No

Reviewer #2:No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool,https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS atgro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: review PONE-D-23-05362.pdf

  • PLoS One. 2023; 18(7): e0288451.
  • »
  • Author response to Decision Letter 0

2023; 18(7): e0288451.

Published online 2023 Jul 26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0288451.r002

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

27 Jun 2023

Specific responses have been provided in an uploaded letter titled "Response to Reviewers"

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response Letter.docx

  • PLoS One. 2023; 18(7): e0288451.
  • »
  • Decision Letter 1

2023; 18(7): e0288451.

Published online 2023 Jul 26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0288451.r003

Peter Karl Jonason, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

28 Jun 2023

Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them

PONE-D-23-05362R1

Dear Dr. Mifsud,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at gro.solp@gnillibrohtua.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

Kind regards,

Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

  • PLoS One. 2023; 18(7): e0288451.
  • »
  • Acceptance letter

2023; 18(7): e0288451.

Published online 2023 Jul 26. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0288451.r004

Peter Karl Jonason, Academic Editor

Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

3 Jul 2023

PONE-D-23-05362R1

Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them

Dear Dr. Mifsud:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at gro.solp@enosolp.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Peter Karl Jonason

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

Worldviews and the role of social values that underlie them (2024)

FAQs

What are the world views and values? ›

Worldviews are essentially the way that people use their values in a consistent manner to act on information about the environment. The perceived structure can be highly tinted by the cognitive tools they use to collect information and the metaphors that they use for comparison.

What role do worldviews play in your understanding of social work practice? ›

Social work values, practice theories, assessments, intervention de- cisions, and action strategies are all shaped by our worldview assumptions and our beliefs.

What is the role of the worldview? ›

Worldviews encompass beliefs that shape our outlook on life and they are pertinent in providing meaning to our reality and our understanding of how we fit within it [10].

What is an example of a worldview? ›

Worldviews tend to include beliefs about who deserves good things (or who most deserves them). Examples: Socialists think that most people deserve good things, but workers in particular deserve to reap the rewards of their labor; capitalists who exploit others are less deserving.

What are the three main worldviews? ›

There are three main worldviews: human-centered, life-centered, and earth-centered. A human-centered environmental worldview sees the planet as a support system for human life and holds that we can and should manage the world for our benefit.

How do you explain your worldview? ›

It could include your perceptions on such things as your view of politics, philosophy, and religion. We may not stop to think about it too often, but it is really our worldview that shapes how we interact with the world, and it greatly influences our values, choices and actions.

How important worldviews are to our lives? ›

Our world view determines our values. It helps us interpret the world around us. It sorts out what is important from what is not, what is of highest value from what is least.” In future articles we'll consider how worldviews function in a way that give meaning, purpose, and clarity to our lives.

How is a worldview a view for life? ›

Your worldview is the philosophy of your life that shapes your world. It structures your beliefs which in turn forms the foundation of your values. Values then influence the choices you make and consequently result in the actions you take.

Why does worldview matter? ›

A biblical worldview not only shapes what a child believes but also equips the child to live in a manner worthy of the gospel, for the good of others and the glory of God through their future vocation.

What best describes a worldview? ›

A worldview is the set of beliefs about fundamental aspects of Reality that ground and influence all one's perceiving, thinking, knowing, and doing. One's worldview is also referred to as one's philosophy, philosophy of life, mindset, outlook on life, formula for life, ideology, faith, or even religion.

What is a word that describes a worldview? ›

perspective position view viewpoint Weltanschauung.

What are all the world views? ›

An overview of the four worldviews
Traditional worldviewModern worldviewPostmodern worldview
Theistic, dualistic, transcendent view of realityObjectified, materialistic, mechanistic view of realityPluralistic, fragmented, relativistic view of reality
5 more rows

What are world view beliefs? ›

A worldview is a collection of attitudes, values, stories and expectations about the world around us, which inform our every thought and action. Worldview is expressed in ethics, religion, philosophy, scientific beliefs and so on (Sire, 2004). A worldview is how a culture works out in individual practice.

What are the four types of worldviews? ›

Creswell and Creswell (2018) identify four worldviews that can help ground your research approach: Postpositivism, Constructivism, Transformative, and Pragmatism. Further discussion of these interpretive lenses in the world of research and how they apply to inquiry are below.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Barbera Armstrong

Last Updated:

Views: 6313

Rating: 4.9 / 5 (79 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Barbera Armstrong

Birthday: 1992-09-12

Address: Suite 993 99852 Daugherty Causeway, Ritchiehaven, VT 49630

Phone: +5026838435397

Job: National Engineer

Hobby: Listening to music, Board games, Photography, Ice skating, LARPing, Kite flying, Rugby

Introduction: My name is Barbera Armstrong, I am a lovely, delightful, cooperative, funny, enchanting, vivacious, tender person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.